Skip to main content

Adapting prenatal iron supplementation to maternal needs results in optimal child neurodevelopment: a follow-up of the ECLIPSES Study

Abstract

Background

Prenatal prescription of standard iron supplements to prevent iron deficiency appears not to be appropriate for all women and their children, as some women may be at risk of iron deficiency and others at risk of iron excess early in pregnancy. The present study aimed to assess whether prenatal iron supplementation adapted to the needs of each pregnant woman affects their child’s neurodevelopment.

Methods

Follow-up of a community-based RCT involving 503 mother–child pairs. Non-anaemic pregnant women recruited in Tarragona (Spain) early in pregnancy were prescribed a daily iron dose based on their initial haemoglobin levels: Stratum 1 (Hb = 110–130 g/L, 80 or 40 mg/d of iron) and Stratum 2 (Hb > 130 g/L, 40 or 20 mg/d of iron). Women receiving 40 mg/d were considered the control group in each Strata. The child’s neurodevelopment was assessed at 40 days of age using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-III (BSID-III). Adjusted multiple regression models were used.

Results

Multiple regression analyses showed no association between the intervention and control group within each Strata on the BSID-III scores on any of the developmental scales in children, including cognitive, language, and motor development: Stratum 1 (β 1.46, 95%CI -2.15, 5.07; β 1.30, 95%CI -1.99, 4.59; and β 2.04, 95%CI -3.88, 7.96, respectively) and Stratum 2 (β -4.04, 95%CI -7.27, 0.80; β -0.36, 95%CI -3.47, 2.75; and β -3.76, 95%CI -9.30, 1.78, respectively).

Conclusions

In non-anaemic women in early pregnancy, no differences were found in the cognitive, language and motor development of children at 40 days of age between the dose of iron tested in each case –adjusted to initial Hb levels– compared to the dose of the control group. Further studies are guaranteed to confirm our findings.

Trial registration

The ECLIPSES study was registered at www.clinicaltrialsregister.eu as EudraCT number 2012–005,480-28.

Peer Review reports

Background

Accumulating evidence indicates that maternal iron status during the gestational period is of great importance for mother–child health [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. The wide variety of processes in which iron is involved means that prenatal iron deficiency might negatively affect the child’s development and, especially, brain functioning [8,9,10]. However, current evidence has mostly originated from animal studies and observational studies in humans. Only a few randomized controlled trials (RCT) have been conducted to evaluate the association between maternal iron status and the child’s cognitive and motor performance. Nevertheless, some important findings have arisen from these studies. In fact, since the harmful effects of prenatal suboptimal iron status have been observed in the short term [11], they could also persist even after correcting iron deficit [12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. Thus, iron supplements are usually prescribed to pregnant women [19,20,21], with good results in improving the serum iron-related biomarkers during gestation, [22,23,24,25] although they have not been always associated with better developmental outcomes for their children [26,27,28]. Also, it should be pointed out the risk of iron overload when an iron-replete woman or those with mutations in the HFE gene –which increases the intestinal iron absorption, especially in homozygotes but also in heterozygosis to a lesser extent– receives routine prenatal iron supplements [29,30,31], as well as the negative consequences that have also been associated with prenatal excess iron on the neuropsychological functions of the child [14, 22, 25, 32, 33]. In this regard, it is important to highlight that the overall prevalence of HFE gene alterations in our population is quite high [34]. After some authors have concluded that both iron deficit and excess may injure the child’s cognitive and motor development [32, 35, 36], they advise that prenatal iron supplementation should be individualized considering maternal iron stores, as well as other lifestyle and biological conditions [37, 38]. We supported this advice after having shown by a community-based Randomized Clinical Trial (RCT) the effectiveness of adapting iron doses to maternal haemoglobin (Hb) levels early in pregnancy in preventing iron deficiency, anaemia, and haemoconcentration [39]. [34, 40]In this study, the obtained results were adjusted for iron stores and specific HFE genotypes, among other women’s characteristics, demonstrating that Hb levels in early pregnancy (at gestational week 12) were related to serum ferritin concentrations and abnormalities in the HFE gene. Like ours, some authors advocated personalizing iron supplements used during pregnancy to provide the most appropriate supply of iron for each woman, which helps to prevent iron deficiency in some women and iron excess in others, improving the maternal iron status during pregnancy in all of them [22, 23, 25, 41]. However, little research has focused on the effects of this prenatal iron supplementation on the child’s neurocognitive abilities, and the available studies show inconsistent results [33, 42,43,44].

We have already shown in our study population that adapting prenatal iron supplementation (80, 40, and 20 mg) to early pregnancy iron levels (normal, Hb 110–130 g/L, or normal-high Hb > 130 g/L) in non-anaemic women, prevents iron imbalances during pregnancy, both due to iron deficiency and excess [39]. Specifically, we found that in women at risk of iron deficiency (those with normal Hb levels at the beginning of pregnancy), a daily dose of 80 mg of iron reduced the risk of iron deficiency, compared to women in the control group receiving 40 mg iron daily, without increasing the risk of excess of iron that can be caused by supplementation with high doses. Similarly, in women at risk of haemoconcentration (those with basal normal-high Hb levels), the tested iron dose of 20 mg daily prevented haemoconcentration compared to the control group, without causing iron deficiency. As a step forward, the present study aimed to evaluate the association between different prenatal doses of iron and the child’s neurodevelopment under the hypothesis that having corrected the women’s iron status by adapting prenatal iron supplementation to their individual needs would also have beneficial effects on the child’s neurodevelopment.

Methods

Study design and data collection

The present work is the follow-up of the ECLIPSES study, a community-based RCT that aimed to assess the effectiveness of different doses of prenatal iron supplementation on the maternal iron status at the end of gestation, and now, on the child’s neurodevelopment. The ECLIPSES study was conducted in the province of Tarragona (Catalonia, Spain) on women recruited before gestational week 12 between 2013–2017 and allocated into two groups according to their Hb levels aiming to prevent an iron deficit in women from the Stratum 1 (initial Hb = 110–130 g/L) and risk of developing iron excess in those from the Stratum 2 (initial Hb > 130 g/L). We used Hb levels as a screening biomarker because they are usually related to iron stores and HFE gene mutations [34, 40]. Women in Stratum 1 were randomly prescribed a daily dose of 40 or 80 mg of iron supplements, while those in Stratum 2 received 40 or 20 mg of iron daily (Fig. 1). Since previous literature has already made it clear that prenatal iron supplementation is recommended and leads to better pregnancy outcomes than non-supplementation, we did not include a control group of non-supplemented women. Otherwise, women in each Strata receiving 40 mg/day, which is the commonly prescribed dose of iron, were considered the control group against which the higher and lower iron dose interventions were performed. The prescription was triple-blinded, so neither the supplement providers, the health workers, nor the researchers knew what dose of iron each woman received until the end of the study. The women were instructed to take one pill a day. At the next study visit, they were to return any leftover pills to assess compliance. This was done by comparing the number of leftover pills with the participants' self-reported compliance. Compliance was considered good when women had forgotten to take the supplement less than twice a week while adherence was considered low when they had forgotten two or more times a week at any of the study visits.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the study. Hb, haemoglobin; GW, gestational week

The ECLIPSES study was registered at www.clinicaltrialsregister.eu as EudraCT number 2012–005,480-28 and its methodological details can be found extended elsewhere [39, 45].

Women were visited in the first, second, and third trimesters of pregnancy, and on average at 40 days post-partum. A summary of maternal information recorded by midwives from questionnaires was as follows:

  • Clinical and obstetrical history: maternal age, parity, pregnancy planning.

  • Anthropometric measurements: weight and height. Body mass index (BMI, kg/m2) was calculated.

  • Dietary assessment: self-administered food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) previously validated in our population [46]. Participants reported usual food consumption retrospectively at weeks 12, 24, and 36 of pregnancy and 40 days post-partum. The FFQ consisted of 45 items classified into 12 food groups: 1–read and processed meat, 2.–poultry, fish, and eggs, 3.–fruits, 4.–vegetables, 5.–dairy products, 6.–salted cereals (breakfast cereals, bread, pasta, and rice), 7.–sweet cereals (biscuits, pastries), 8.–legumes, 9.–nuts, 10.–sweets, 11.–sweetened beverages, 12.–alcoholic drinks. The FFQ data were reviewed and analysed by trained nutritionists, who calculated the intake of each food group in grams/day. Additionally, the women’s degree of adherence to the Mediterranean diet was calculated using an rMED score based on the intake of 9 components of this diet. Each rMED component (apart from alcohol) was expressed in grams per 1000 kcal/day (to express intake as energy density) and was divided by terciles of dietary intake. Each tercile was assigned a value of 0, 1, and 2 points. Out of the 9 components of the rMED, 6 of them (fruit, vegetables, legumes, cereals, fresh fish and seafood, and olive oil) scored positively, while 2 scored negatively (total and processed meat, dairy products). Alcohol was scored as a dichotomous variable (0 for women who consumed alcohol, and 2 for women who did not drink alcohol). The score assigned to each pregnant woman thus ranged from 0 points indicating minimum adherence to 18 points indicating maximum adherence to the Mediterranean diet. The total rMED score was classified into three categories: 0–6 it was considered as ‘‘low’’, 7–10 as ‘‘medium’’, and 11–18 as ‘‘high’’. Extended information can be found in Jardí et al. [47].

  • Lifestyle at the time of recruitment: use of prenatal supplements other than iron, smoking habit (using the Fagerström test [48]), and physical activity (using the short form of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire [IPAQ] [49]). The IPAQ assesses physical activity considering the following domains: leisure time, domestic activities, work-related physical activity, and transport-related physical activity. Within these domains, the IPAQ short form focus on walking, moderate-intensity activities, and vigorous-intensity activities. Total scores are computed based on the duration (in minutes) and frequency (in days) of each type of activity. The IPAQ offers the specific algorithms to obtain the classification of “low”, “moderate”, and “high” physical activity by combining the duration and frequency of different types of activities.Sociodemographic characteristics: ethnic origin, familiar socioeconomic status (SES) calculated from educational level and occupational status both from participants and their partners.

  • Maternal anxiety status: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [50]. The STAI test assessed two separate concepts of anxiety, each with 20 items. On one hand, anxiety as a state assesses a transient emotional state, characterised by subjective, consciously perceived feelings of alertness and apprehension and by hyperactivity of the autonomic nervous system. On the other hand, anxiety as a trait indicates a relatively stable anxious propensity that characterises individuals with a tendency to perceive situations as threatening.Post-partum depression: Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [51].

Blood samples were collected in 2 tubes of 7.5 ml, one containing EDTA as anticoagulant and the other without anticoagulant. The samples were transported to the BioBank for immediate analyses. Before processing, the samples in the EDTA tubes were inversion-mixed 10 times to ensure that the blood was mixed, then centrifuged at 4°C to separate plasma. The tube without anticoagulant was left without mixing for 30 minutes at room temperature to enable coagulation, then the serum was separated also by centrifugation. All the samples were stored at -80°C. DNA was extracted and stored as well at -80°C for subsequent genetic analyses. The stored samples in the BioBank were thawed at the end of the clinical study and processed simultaneously to minimize inter-batch variation. Biochemical determinations of Hb, serum ferritin (SF) and cortisol were done by immunochemiluminescence at each trimester of pregnancy. The serum concentration of C-reactive protein (CRP) was measured by immunoturbidimetry also at each trimester of pregnancy. Plasma polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA) concentrations were analysed by using a combination of gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (GC-MS) after their derivatization to methyl ester (FAMEs) due to their higher volatility. [52] Detailed information on laboratory procedures for PUFA measurements can be found in Aparicio et al. [53] Serum concentrations of vitamin D were quantified by an automated chemiluminescent immunoassay method as described in Díaz-López et al. [54] The rationale for including serum concentrations of vitamin D and fatty acids in the analyses is that these components are involved in brain development, so maternal levels during pregnancy may influence foetal neurodevelopment. [55,56,57] Folate and vitamin B12 measurements were also done by using a chemiluminescence immunoassay only in the first trimester of gestation. Then, RBC folate concentration was calculated as follows: (serum folate in haemolysed whole blood * dilution factor in haemolysis * 100)/haematocrit. [58] Genetic analyses to detect mutations in the HFE gene were done using polymerase chain reaction (PCR) and digestion with specific enzymes.

As for the infants’ information, the following data were recorded at birth: sex, gestational age (calculated based on the time elapsed since the first day of the last self-reported menstrual period), Apgar test score, type of feeding, and anthropometric measurements including length, weight, and head circumference. At 40 days of age, children were visited again and information about the type of feeding as well as weight and height measurements were recorded that time.

Outcome

The individualized assessment of the child neurodevelopment was performed by two trained psychologists in the facilities of the health care centre participating in the study at the average age of 40 days using The Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 3rd edition (BSID-III) [59]. This test consists of three general scales (cognitive, language, and motor) obtaining a standardized IQ score (mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15) and four subscales (expressive language, receptive language, fine motor, and gross motor) obtaining a standardized scalar score (mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3). Higher scores represent better development.Continuous BSID-III scores for general scales were categorized as follows according to the test rates: low (scores < 85), middle (scores ≥ 85–115), and high (scores ≥ 115). Similarly, the classification was as follows for subscales: low (scores < 7), middle (scores ≥ 7–13), and high (scores ≥ 13). Given the low number of children in the “high” category in cognitive and language scales, they were merged with those in the “middle” category when performing logistic regression analysis. In the case of the motor scale, children in the “low” category were merged with those in the “middle” category for the same reason.

Statistical analyses

The analyses were considered as per protocol analyses and were stratified according to the design by Hb concentration category at baseline.. Bivariate analyses to describe the variables of interest were performed using the conventional statistical techniques: Student T and ANOVA tests for continuous variables using mean and standard deviation (SD) and Chi-square test of percentages for categorical ones. Natural logarithm (Ln) transformation was applied to normalize the distribution of SF, increasing the validity of analyses.

Linear and logistic regression models were used to assess the effect of different doses of prenatal iron supplementation (Stratum 1: 80 mg vs control, Stratum 2: 20 mg vs control) on the child’s neurodevelopment. Crude and adjusted estimates are shown. Based on previous knowledge, the models were adjusted for those maternal and child variables that could affect the studied relationship as follows: maternal age at recruitment, parity (yes or no), pregnancy planning (yes or no), familiar socioeconomic status (low, middle, high), smoking at recruitment (yes or no), baseline maternal BMI (normal weight, overweight, obesity), gestational weight gain, maternal anxiety during pregnancy, post-partum depression, serum levels of Hb, ferritin, vitamin D and polyunsaturated fatty acids at the first and third trimester of pregnancy, serum levels of RBC folate and vitamin B12 at the first trimester of pregnancy, physical activity during pregnancy (low, moderate, high), adherence to the Mediterranean diet (low, middle, high) and daily energy intake at the first trimester of pregnancy, child’s age at assessment, sex, gestational age, Apgar test scores (< 7 or ≥ 7 points), and head circumference at birth.

The statistical analyses were done using the SPSS software (version 27.0 for Windows; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Ethical approval

The study was designed in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki/Tokyo. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Jordi Gol University Institute for Primary Care Research [Institut d’Investigació en Atenció Primària; IDIAP], the Pere Virgili Health Research Institute [Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili; IISPV] and of the Spanish Agency for Medicines and Medical Devices [Agencia Española del Medicamento y Productos Sanitarios; AEMPS]. Signed informed consent was obtained from all women participating in the study.

The quality of the present community-based RCT has been assessed by the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT).

Results

The current analyses consisted of 503 mother–child pairs from the ECLIPSES study, from which data on child neurodevelopment assessment at 40 days of age was available. Table 1 summarizes the maternal baseline characteristics, being remarkable that the median ± interquartile range maternal age was 31 ± 7 years, that near of 40% were overweight or obese 14.7% were smokers at the conceptional time, and most of them were White, had low or middle SES, and had planned the pregnancy. We also found that more than half of women (61.2%) showed low-middle adherence to the Mediterranean diet and moderate physical activity during pregnancy (66.9%). There was a high compliance to the intervention throughout pregnancy (around 94%). No association was found regarding sociodemographic characteristics and lifestyle between participants whose data were included or not included in the present analyses (Supplementary Table 1).

Table 1 Maternal characteristics

Maternal concentrations of iron-related biomarkers in the first and third trimesters are shown in Supplementary Table 2. As for the maternal iron status, since anaemia was an exclusion criterion for the recruitment and haemoconcentration is a condition associated with late pregnancy, only data for the third trimester of gestation are shown. The participants showed a low prevalence of iron-deficiency anaemia in all the iron groups (1 to 5%), without any association among them. As for haemoconcentration, the overall prevalence was 13.7% with the higher percentage being shown by women in Stratum 2. Additional information on maternal levels of vitamin B12 and RBC folate at the beginning of pregnancy are also shown in Supplementary Table 2.

The child’s characteristics according to the intervention group are depicted in Table 2. The results showed that 49.5% of the participating children were girls, had a mean gestational age of 39.7 weeks, and a median age of 47 ± 14 days when neurodevelopment was assessed. Regarding the BSID-III scores, they were normally distributed in our study population and the mean scores obtained for all the children were in the normal ranges at each scale when they were analysed as a continuous variable. However, when the scores were categorized considering the clinical cut-off point for normality (85 or 7 points for main scales and subscales, respectively), a small percentage of children were under the normality for language development (8.1%, n = 45) and, specifically, for expressive language development (11.9%, n = 65). No association was found in the BSID-III scores or in the percentage of children below the cut-off point considered normal, between the different doses of iron in any Strata.

Table 2 Characteristics of children according to the dose of maternal iron supplementation during pregnancy

In multiple linear regression analysis, no association was found between prenatally prescribed iron doses in Stratum 1 or 2 about BSID-III scores on any of the developmental scales in children at 40 days of age, including cognitive, language, and motor development (Table 3, Fig. 2). Neither association was found between the doses of iron in any of the groups in the logistic regression analysis on the chances of moving from low to medium–high mental and language development, and from low-medium to high motor development (Table 3, Fig. 2).

Table 3 Effect of iron supplementation on the neurodevelopment of children at around 40 days of life
Fig. 2
figure 2

Effect of iron supplementation on the neurodevelopment of children at around 40 days of life. The control group in each Stratum were women who received the commonly prescribed dose of 40 mg/d of iron. Models adjusted for maternal age at recruitment, parity, pregnancy planning, familiar socioeconomic status, smoking at recruitment, baseline maternal body mass index, gestational weight gain, maternal anxiety during pregnancy, postpartum depression, serum biomarker levels at the first and third trimester of pregnancy (haemoglobin, ferritin, vitamin D and polyunsaturated fatty acids), serum biomarker levels at the first trimester of pregnancy (red blood cell folate and vitamin B12), physical activity during pregnancy, adherence to Mediterranean diet and daily energy intake at the first trimester of pregnancy, child’s age at assessment, child’s sex, gestational age, Apgar test scores, head circumference at birth, and type of feeding at birth and assessment. Odds Ratios express the chance to go from low to middle-high cognitive development and language development, and from low-middle to high motor development

Discussion

The need for preventive prenatal iron supplementation to avoid iron deficiency is well known, although favourable results on the child’s development have not always been observed. Routine preventive supplementation with standard doses of iron for all women could probably be insufficient for those who start pregnancy with low iron levels and too much for those with high initial iron concentrations. Thus, iron supplementation adapted to maternal needs would prevent both extremes and, in turn, improve child outcomes. This community-based study found that adapting prenatal iron supplementation in non-anaemic women to their individual needs to prevent iron deficit and excess, led to similar results in their children's cognitive, language, and motor development in all iron supplementation groups (daily 20 mg, 80 mg, and control group of 40 mg), resulting in a lack of association between the tested doses and the control group in each Strata for child’s neurodevelopment.

Little research assessing the effect of prenatal iron supplementation on the child’s neurodevelopment has been conducted on well-nourished non-anaemic women, and studies until now only evaluated the effect of taking or not prenatal iron supplements, without considering different doses according to the individual needs of each woman. Results from the AMBIT study [26, 27], conducted in non-anaemic Australian pregnant, indicated no effects of prenatal iron supplementation (20 mg daily) compared to placebo on the offspring’s intelligence and behavioural skills at 4 and 6–8 years of age. Some years later, a study conducted in 9-months-old Chinese children assessing the effectiveness of antenatal (60 mg daily vs placebo) and infant iron supplementation found an improvement in the child’s gross motor development, although it was not due to iron supplementation during pregnancy, but during infancy [28]. Despite the tested doses in these studies being around the usual doses in routine supplementation, considering that the participating women were not anaemic, they could have been inadequate depending on their iron stores and other factors. In this regard, women with homozygous mutations in the HFE gene, which increase iron absorption, may be at risk of iron overload if they receive iron supplements [29,30,31]. Despite the low prevalence of homozygosis for HFE gene mutations in our study population (only 4.8% for H63D genotype), the prevalence of having any HFE gene mutation is around 46% in the Mediterranean population [34, 60], which turn it into an important risk factor to consider when prescribing iron supplementation. There are some indications from observational studies that not only iron deficiency but also iron excess negatively affects the child’s neurodevelopment [18, 32, 33, 35, 36] and, based on that, some main researchers in this field have proposed to adapt the prenatal iron supplementation to the individual’s requirements to mitigate the potential damage from any maternal iron imbalance [22, 23, 25, 41]. However, few studies have tested this hypothesis, obtaining inconsistent results [37, 61]. We observed that our intervention successfully corrected maternal iron status when compared with the estimates of the prevalence of anaemia and haemoconcentration during pregnancy. While the prevalence of anaemia in Europe is around 25% in pregnant women [19, 62, 63], we found that only 3.3–5% of participants at risk developed it at the end of pregnancy. On the other hand, despite the estimates indicating that up to 42% of women suffer from haemoconcentration in industrialized countries [25, 64], we observed a prevalence of 15.6–25.6% among participants at risk of iron excess. We believe, therefore, that having provided the most appropriate amount of iron for each woman helping them to reach an optimal iron status in most cases has been the physiological mechanism underlying the lack of a remarkable association between different prenatal iron doses and child’s neurodevelopment. We found that only 2.6%, 8.1%, and 3% of children obtained scores below the normal range for cognitive, language, and motor development scales, respectively. The high heterogeneity among the epidemiological studies assessing the effect of prenatal iron supplementation on the child’s neurodevelopment makes it difficult to compare our findings. Nonetheless, some evidence from observational studies indicate that failure to prevent women from suffering from both iron deficiency and excess in pregnancy results in neurodevelopmental impairment in children. That is the case of two Spanish studies [11, 18] and others from Vietnam [65] and China [66] from which the authors concluded that iron deficiency or anaemia in late pregnancy, compared with having a correct iron status, may be associated with lower motor scores in young children. Similarly, maternal iron deficiency during pregnancy can result in poorer cognitive and language abilities in children, according to some of those studies [18, 67]. But, as previously discussed, that prenatal iron excess could entails harmful consequences for child neurodevelopment has also been stated in the literature, especially associated with cognitive function in this case [32, 36, 68]. And, going further, a couple of studies made sense of the present work, showing in their study population that the association between maternal iron status and child neurodevelopment is sometimes inverted U-shaped [69, 70]. Considering the available evidence, our results suggest that preventive prenatal supplementation with different doses of iron as long as they are in a range appropriate to each woman's needs, i.e. adapted to their initial Hb levels, lead to similar neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants at birth. The main strengths of the present work were the study design, which was a community-based triple-blinded community-based RCT and the extensive data collection regarding sociodemographic conditions, clinical and lifestyle information from both mothers and children. Also, It should be noted that women in our study were non-anaemic when they were recruited in the first trimester of pregnancy and started iron supplementation. This does not mean that they cannot become anaemic during pregnancy. However, by adjusting for maternal serum ferritin and Hb concentrations in both the first and third trimesters of pregnancy, we were able to rule out the possible effect of maternal iron status and estimate the true effect of prenatal iron supplementation on the child’s neurodevelopment. However, some limitations should also be considered. First, despite being common in community-based intervention studies implying several visits and a long follow-up, the substantial drop-out that occurred could have reduced the statistical precision and may have led to biased estimates of intervention effects. Second, although the BSID-III is an internationally used and recognised tool for assessing the child’s cognitive function, the neurodevelopmental assessment shows low stability in early childhood [71], which could have led to estimates of nullity. And finally, residual confounding, due to unmeasured or unknown risk factors that may occur even after adjustment for known potential confounders, could have been a limitation when interpreting our findings.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that in non-anaemic women at the start of pregnancy, there were no differences in the cognitive, language and motor development of children at 40 days of age between the dose of iron tested in each case (80 or 20 mg/d) –adjusted to initial Hb levels– compared to the dose of the control group (40 mg/d), which is the dose commonly used in clinical practice in SpainThe research in this regard is still scarce and further studies are guaranteed to better understand the possible effects of different types of prenatal iron supplementation on the child’s neurodevelopment, including the follow-up at older ages.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

RCT:

Randomized clinical trial

Hb:

Haemoglobin

BMI:

Body mass index

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

SES:

Socioeconomic status

STAI:

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

EPDS:

Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale

SF:

Serum ferritin

RBC:

Red blood cell

BSID:

Bayley Scales of Infant Development

SD:

Standard deviation

Ln:

Natural logarithm

References

  1. Rayman MP, Barlis J, Evans RW, Redman CWG, King LJ. Abnormal iron parameters in the pregnancy syndromeclampsia. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002;187:412–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Rahmati S, Azami M, Parizad N, Sayehmiri K. The relationship between maternal anemia during pregnancy with preterm birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Matern-Fetal Neonatal Med. 2020;33:2679–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Figueiredo ACMG, Gomes-Filho IS, Silva RB, Pereira PPS, Da MFAF, Lyrio AO, et al. Maternal anemia and low birth weight: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2018;10:610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ribot B, Aranda N, Viteri FE, Hernández-Martínez C, Canals J, Arija V. Depleted iron stores without anaemia early in pregnancy carries increased risk of lower birthweight even when supplemented daily with moderate iron. Hum Reprod. 2012;27:1260–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Gutierrez-Aguirre CH, García-Lozano JA, Treviño-Montemayor OR, Iglesias-Benavides JL, Cantú-Rodríguez OG, González-Llano O, et al. Comparative analysis of iron status and other hematological parameters in preeclampsia. Hematol. 2017;22:36–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Díaz-López A, Ribot B, Basora J, Arija V. High and low Haemoglobin levels in early pregnancy are associated to a higher risk of miscarriage: a population-based cohort study. Nutrients. 2021;13:1578.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Scholl TO. Iron status during pregnancy : setting the stage for mother and infant. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;81:1218S-1222S.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. The role of nutrition in brain development: the golden opportunity of the “first 1000 days.” J Pediatr. 2016;175:16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. McCann S, Amadó MP, Moore SE. The role of iron in brain development: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2020;12:2001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Mattei D, Pietrobelli A. Micronutrients and brain development. Curr Nutr Rep. 2019;8:99–107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Hernández-Martínez C, Canals J, Aranda N, Ribot B, Escribano J, Arija V. Effects of iron deficiency on neonatal behavior at different stages of pregnancy. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87:165–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Vallée L. Fer et neurodéveloppement. Archives de Pediatrie. 2017;24:5S18-22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Radlowski EC, Johnson RW. Perinatal iron deficiency and neurocognitive development. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:585.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Gaillard R, Eilers PHC, Yassine S, Hofman A, Steegers EAP, Jaddoe VWV. Risk factors and consequences of maternal anaemia and elevated haemoglobin levels during pregnancy: a population-based prospective cohort study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2014;28:213–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Beard J. Iron deficiency alters brain development and functioning. J Nutr. 2003;133:1468S-1472S.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Todorich B, Pasquini JM, Garcia CI, Paez PM, Connor JR. Oligodendrocytes and myelination: the role of iron. Glia. 2009;57:467–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK, Rao R. Approaches for reducing the risk of early-life iron deficiency-induced brain dysfunction in children. Nutrients. 2018;10:227.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Berglund SK, Torres-Espínola FJ, García-Valdés L, Segura MT, Martínez-Zaldívar C, Padilla C, et al. The impacts of maternal iron deficiency and being overweight during pregnancy on neurodevelopment of the offspring. Br J Nutr. 2017;118:533–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Stevens GA, Finucane MM, De-Regil LM, Paciorek CJ, Flaxman SR, Branca F, et al. Global, regional, and national trends in haemoglobin concentration and prevalence of total and severe anaemia in children and pregnant and non-pregnant women for 1995–2011: A systematic analysis of population-representative data. Lancet Glob Health. 2013;1:16–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. World Health Organization, Williams AL, van Drongelen W, Lasky RE, Sanderson M, Lai D, et al. Guideline : Daily iron and folic acid supplementation in pregnant women, vol. 46 . Geneva: World Health Organization; 2012. p. 323–9.

  21. World Health Organization. WHO Recommendation on Antenatal care for positive pregnancy experience. Geneva: WHO Recommendation on Antenatal care for positive pregnancy experience; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Casanueva E, Viteri FE, Mares-Galindo M, Meza-Camacho C, Loría A, Schnaas L, et al. Weekly iron as a safe alternative to daily supplementation for nonanemic pregnant women. Arch Med Res. 2006;37:674–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Milman N. Oral iron prophylaxis in pregnancy: not too little and not too much! J Pregnancy. 2012;2012:514345.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Milman N. Iron prophylaxis in pregnancy - General or individual and in which dose? Ann Hematol. 2006;85:821–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Peña-Rosas JP, Viteri FE. Effects and safety of preventive oral iron or iron+folic acid supplementation for women during pregnancy. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009;7:CD004736.

  26. Parsons AG, Zhou SJ, Spurrier NJ, Makrides M. Effect of iron supplementation during pregnancy on the behaviour of children at early school age: long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr. 2008;99:1133–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Zhou SJ, Gibson RA, Crowther CA, Baghurst P, Makrides M. Effect of iron supplementation during pregnancy on the intelligence quotient and behavior of children at 4 y of age: long-term follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:1112–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Angulo-Barroso RM, Li M, Santos DCC, Bian Y, Sturza J, Jiang Y, et al. Iron supplementation in pregnancy or infancy and motor development: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137:e20153547.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Crownover B, Covey C. Hereditary Hemochromatosis. Am Fam Physician. 2013;87:183–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hanson EH, Imperatore G, Burke W. HFE gene and hereditary hemochromatosis: A HuGE review. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154:193–206.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Barton JC, Edwards CQ, Acton RT. HFE gene: Structure, function, mutations, and associated iron abnormalities. Gene. 2015;574:179–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Aranda N, Hernández-Martínez C, Arija V, Ribot B, Canals J. Haemoconcentration risk at the end of pregnancy: effects on neonatal behaviour. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:1405–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Georgieff MK, Krebs NF, Cusick SE. The benefits and risks of iron supplementation in pregnancy and childhood. Ann Rev Nutr. 2019;39:121–46.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Aranda N, Viteri FE, Fernández-Ballart J, Murphy M, Arija V. Frequency of the hemochromatosis gene (HFE) 282C→Y, 63H→D, and 65S→C mutations in a general Mediterranean population from Tarragona. Spain Ann Hematol. 2007;86:17–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Quezada-Pinedo HG, Cassel F, Duijts L, Muckenthaler MU, Gassmann M, Jaddoe VWV, et al. Maternal iron status in pregnancy and child health outcomes after birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2021;13:2221.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Sammallahti S, Tiemeier H, Reiss IKM, Muckenthaler MU, el Marroun H, Vermeulen M. Maternal early-pregnancy ferritin and offspring neurodevelopment: a prospective cohort study from gestation to school age. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2022;36:425–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Iglesias L, Canals J, Arija V. Effects of prenatal iron status on child neurodevelopment and behavior: A systematic review. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2017;58:1604–14.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Aranda N, Ribot B, Viteri FE, Cavallé P, Arija V. Predictors of haemoconcentration at delivery: association with low birth weight. Eur J Nutr. 2013;52:1631–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Iglesias-Vázquez L, Arija V, Aranda N, Aparicio E, Serrat N, Fargas F, et al. The effectiveness of different doses of iron supplementation and the prenatal determinants of maternal iron status in pregnant Spanish women: ECLIPSES study. Nutrients. 2019;11:2418.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Beutler E, Felitti V, Gelbart T, Ho N. The effect of HFE genotypes on measurements of iron overload in patients attending a health appraisal clinic. Ann Intern Med. 2000;133:329–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Milman N, Byg KE, Bergholt T, Eriksen L, Hvas AM. Body iron and individual iron prophylaxis in pregnancy - Should the iron dose be adjusted according to serum ferritin? Ann Hematol. 2006;85:567–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Jayasinghe C, Polson R, van Woerden HC, Wilson P. The effect of universal maternal antenatal iron supplementation on neurodevelopment in offspring: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Pediatr. 2018;18:1–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Larson LM, Phiri KS, Pasricha SR. Iron and Cognitive Development: What Is the Evidence? Ann Nutr Metab. 2017;71:25–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Saint SE, Frick JE. Prenatal Supplementation and Its Effects on Early Childhood Cognitive Outcome. In: Wallace TC, editor. Dietary Supplements in Health Promotion. CRC Press; 2015. p. 88–117.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Arija V, Fargas F, March G, Abajo S, Basora J, Canals J, et al. Adapting iron dose supplementation in pregnancy for greater effectiveness on mother and child health: protocol of the ECLIPSES randomized clinical trial. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2014;14:33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Trinidad Rodríguez I, Fernández Ballart J, Cucó Pastor G, Biarnés Jordà E, Arija VV. Validation of a short questionnaire on frequency of dietary intake: reproducibility and validity. Nutr Hosp. 2008;23:242–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jardí C, Aparicio E, Bedmar C, Aranda N, Abajo S, March G, et al. Food consumption during pregnancy and post-partum ECLIPSES Study. Nutrients. 2019;11(10):2447.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Fagerström KO. Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addict Behav. 1978;3:235–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. The IPAQ Group. International Physical Activity Questionnaire. IPAQ Website. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL LR. STAI Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado Rasgo. (Adaptación española: Nicolás Seisdedos Cubero). 1997.

  51. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. David F, Tienpont B, Klee MS, Tripp P. Automated Sample Preparation for Profiling Fatty Acids in Blood and Plasma Using the Agilent 7693. Agil Appl Note. 5990‑4822E. Agilent. 2009.

  53. Aparicio E, Martín-Grau C, Hernández-Martinez C, Voltas N, Canals J, Arija V. Changes in fatty acid levels (saturated, monounsaturated and polyunsaturated) during pregnancy. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2021;21:778.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Díaz-López A, Jardí C, Villalobos M, Serrat N, Basora J, Arija V. Prevalence and risk factors of hypovitaminosis D in pregnant Spanish women. Sci Rep. 2020;10:15757.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Voltas N, Canals J, Hernández-Martínez C, Serrat N, Basora J, Arija V. Effect of vitamin d status during pregnancy on infant neurodevelopment: The ECLIPSES study. Nutrients. 2020;12:3196.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Martinat M, Rossitto M, di Miceli M, Layé S. Perinatal dietary polyunsaturated fatty acids in brain development, role in neurodevelopmental disorders. Nutrients. 2021;13:1185.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Zou R, el Marroun H, Voortman T, Hillegers M, White T, Tiemeier H. Maternal polyunsaturated fatty acids during pregnancy and offspring brain development in childhood. Am J Clin Nutr. 2021;114:124–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Iglesias-Vázquez L, Serrat N, Bedmar C, Pallejà-Millán M, Arija V. Prenatal folic acid supplementation and folate status in early pregnancy: ECLIPSES study. Br J Nutr 2021;6:1–8.

  59. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development–Third Edition. San Antonio. 2006.

  60. Aranda N, Viteri FE, Montserrat C, Arija V. Effects of C282Y, H63D, and S65C HFE gene mutations, diet, and life-style factors on iron status in a general Mediterranean population from Tarragona. Spain Ann Hematol. 2010;89:767–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Yadav K, Arjun MC, Jacob OM, Kant S, Ahamed F, Ramaswamy G. Comparison of different doses of daily iron supplementation for anemia prophylaxis in pregnancy: A systematic review. J Family Med Prim Care. 2020;9:1308.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Milman N, Taylor CL, Merkel J, Brannon PM. Iron status in pregnant women and women of reproductive age in Europe. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106:1655S-1662S.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. World Health Organization. Worldwide prevalence of anaemia 1993–2005. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2008.

  64. Arija V, Ribot B, Aranda N. Prevalence of iron deficiency states and risk of haemoconcentration during pregnancy according to initial iron stores and iron supplementation. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16:1371–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Tran TD, Tran T, Simpson JA, Tran HT, Nguyen TT, Hanieh S, et al. Infant motor development in rural Vietnam and intrauterine exposures to anaemia, iron deficiency and common mental disorders: a prospective community-based study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2014;14:8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Chang S, Zeng L, Brouwer ID, Kok FJ, Yan H. Effect of iron deficiency anemia in pregnancy on child mental development in rural China. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e755–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Tran TD, Biggs BA, Tran T, Simpson JA, Hanieh S, Dwyer T, et al. Impact on infants’ cognitive development of antenatal exposure to iron deficiency disorder and common mental disorders. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Yang L, Ren A, Liu J, Ye R, Hong S, Zheng J. Influence of hemoglobin level during early gestation on the development of cognition of pre-school children. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi. 2010;31:1353–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Hou J, Johnston KE, Cliver SP, Ramey SL, et al. Cord serum ferritin concentrations and mental and psychomotor development of children at five years of age. J Pediatr. 2002;140:165–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Mireku MO, Davidson LL, Koura GK, Ouedraogo S, Boivin MJ, Xiong X, et al. Prenatal Hemoglobin Levels and Early Cognitive and Motor Functions of One-Year-Old Children. Pediatrics. 2015;136:e76-83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Kvestad I, Hysing M, Ranjitkar S, Shrestha M, Ulak M, Chandyo RK, et al. The stability of the Bayley scales in early childhood and its relationship with future intellectual abilities in a low to middle income country. Early Hum Dev. 2022;170: 105610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank to the Jordi Gol Research Institute in Primary Care [Institut d’Investigació en Atenció Primària; IDIAP] for their guidance regarding ethical matters. We thank to the entities and participants in the ECLIPSES study: Research Group in Nutrition and Mental Health (NUTRISAM), Universitat Rovira i Virgili (URV), Reus, Spain (Victoria Arija, Josefa Canals, Lucía Iglesias-Vázquez, Cristina Bedmar, Carmen Hernández-Martínez, Cristina Jardí, Núria Voltas); Sexual and Reproductive Health Care Services (ASSIR) of Tarragona, Spain (Francesc Fargas, Francisca Ruiz, Gemma March, Susana Abajo); the team of midwives recruited for the study (Irene Aguilar, Sònia Aguiles, Rosa Alzúria, Judit Bertrán, Carmen Burgos, Elisabet Bru, Montserrat Carreras, Beatriz Fernández, Carme Fonollosa, María Leiva, Demetria Patricio, Teresa Pinto, María Ramírez, Eusebia Romano, Inés Sombreo); the Research support Unit-Tarragona (Josep Basora, Meritxell Pallejà) and the Central Unit-Barcelona (Rosa Morros) of the Institut d’Atenció Primària IDIAP Jordi Gol, Institut Català de la Salut; and the Laboratory of Institut Català de la Salut (ICS), University Hospital of Tarragona Joan XXIII, Tarragona, Spain (Núria Serrat). We thank Pilar Coronel and Mercedes Gimeno of Meiji Pharma Spain S.A. for logistic assistance and for providing iron supplements free of charge. We also thank to the URV and the Diputación de Tarragona for their financial support through the predoctoral fellowship Martí Franquès 2020-PMF-PIPF-10.

Funding

The work was supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Fondo de Investigación Sanitaria, Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo [grant number PI12/02777 and PI17/01754].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

VA conceived and designed the work, obtained funding and resources, participated in the research, as well as in the administration and supervision of the project. JC contributed to the conception and design of the study and participated in the research and supervision of the work. PC and MG provided the iron supplements for the intervention and participated in the research. CH-M and NV conducted the neurodevelopmental assessment of the child and participated in the research process. LI-V performed the formal analyses, contributed to the research and data visualization, and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Victoria Arija.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was designed in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki/Tokyo. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Jordi Gol University Institute for Primary Care Research [Institut d’Investigació en Atenció Primària; IDIAP], the Pere Virgili Health Research Institute [Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili; IISPV] and of the Spanish Agency for Medicines and Medical Devices [Agencia Española del Medicamento y Productos Sanitarios; AEMPS]. Signed informed consent was obtained from all women participating in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors state that they have no conflict of interest to declare.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1. Supplementary Table 1.

Maternal characteristics of participants included and non-included in theanalyses.

Additional file 2. Supplementary Table 2.

Maternal concentrations of iron-related biomarkers, vitamin B12, and RBC folate.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Iglesias-Vázquez, L., Hernández-Martínez, C., Voltas, N. et al. Adapting prenatal iron supplementation to maternal needs results in optimal child neurodevelopment: a follow-up of the ECLIPSES Study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 22, 710 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-022-05033-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-022-05033-y

Keywords